Island foxes evolved bigger brains to help them survive
08-22-2025

Island foxes evolved bigger brains to help them survive

subscribe
facebooklinkedinxwhatsappbluesky

For decades, biologists thought isolation on islands nudged animals toward smaller bodies, tamer behavior, and shrunken brains – an energy-saving suite dubbed “island syndrome.”

However, a new study of Channel Islands foxes complicates that picture. Most of these cat-sized predators evolved relatively larger brains than their mainland gray fox cousins, suggesting that cognitive demands – more than isolation itself – can drive brain size.

The work, led by researchers at the USC Dornsife College of Letters, Arts and Sciences, argues that brain evolution on islands is shaped by the pressures of survival on the ground.

Fox brains break the mold

Lead author Kimberly Schoenberger argued that the results overturn a long-held assumption.

“It was most surprising to discover that island syndrome isn’t one-size-fits-all,” said Schoenberger. “When we looked at carnivores like Channel Island foxes, the pattern of smaller brains didn’t hold.”

That departure matters because most previous brain-size studies focused on island herbivores – animals that face different challenges than hunters and omnivores.

By spotlighting a small carnivore, the team shows that the “rules” of island evolution can fracture along ecological lines.

Six islands, one natural experiment

The Channel Islands – an eight-island chain off Southern California – provide a rare, replicated testbed. Six islands host endemic fox subspecies.

The researchers compared those island foxes to mainland gray foxes, their closest living relatives and probable ancestors, and also contrasted the island subspecies with one another.

If isolation or island area alone drove brain shrinkage, the team would expect a simple pattern. Instead, they found that brain-to-body ratios lined up more closely with local habitat demands than with geography.

On five islands – Santa Cruz, Santa Rosa, Santa Catalina, San Clemente, and San Miguel – the foxes had relatively larger brains despite their smaller bodies.

Those brains showed deeper folds and ridges in regions tied to motor control and spatial processing. Such traits help the animals navigate rugged terrain, remember scattered resources, and compete for scarce shelter.

On two of those islands, foxes share space with rivals like the spotted skunk, raising the bar for fast decisions and agile movement.

Brains shrink when survival is simple

San Nicolas, the most remote and resource-limited island in the study, told the opposite story. There, foxes had smaller relative brains. With few predators, low biodiversity, and a thin menu of foods, the researchers inferred that the foxes faced fewer cognitive demands.

Under those constraints, conserving energy for the basics – staying warm, finding enough calories – may have outweighed the payoff of maintaining larger brain tissue.

“The Channel Island foxes show that brain size reduction is not a universal feature of island life,” Schoenberger said. “It depends on the pressures each species faces.”

Measuring fox minds from bones

To trace brain evolution through time, the team worked with more than 250 skulls representing six island fox subspecies and four gray fox subspecies. These specimens came from the Natural History Museum of Los Angeles County and the Santa Barbara Museum of Natural History.

The researchers first estimated body mass from skull measurements and validated those estimates against recorded weights from live-caught island foxes. That gave them a solid baseline for comparing brain-to-body ratios.

For brain size, they used microbeads to measure braincase volume – a reliable proxy when brains are absent. They then CT-scanned the skulls to build 3D models of the cranial cavities.

The scans confirmed the volume estimates and revealed subtle differences in the surfaces where brain folds press into bone. These patterns allowed the team to infer changes in regions linked to movement and spatial awareness.

Fox brains bend to fit

The digital reconstructions uncovered a telling feature: island foxes had shorter, more compact frontal areas of the braincase than mainland gray foxes. The researchers linked that to the foxes’ shorter snouts, which reduce available space at the front of the skull.

To compensate, the island foxes developed slightly deeper cortical folds and more pronounced ridges in that region. This preserved space for circuitry supporting motor control and navigation – skills especially useful for climbing trees and foraging in complex terrain.

Both island and gray foxes are the only canids known to climb, but island foxes rely on arboreal foraging more often, likely because ground resources are leaner.

The study also found no difference in brain size between males and females. This points away from sexual selection and toward shared environmental pressures as the main driver of change.

Foxes stayed wild with humans

Genetic and radiocarbon evidence suggests foxes first reached the northern Channel Islands roughly 9,000 years ago. They may have arrived by rafting on storm debris or by swimming shorter channels when sea levels were lower.

Thousands of years later, Indigenous peoples may have helped move foxes to other islands – likely as pest control rather than pets.

Despite that long association with humans, island foxes never became truly domesticated. Unlike early domestic dogs, which show brain shrinkage as they grew dependent on people, island foxes retained wild-type behaviors and brain sizes befitting self-sufficient hunters.

Brains matter for survival

As climate change and development carve mainland habitats into island-like fragments, understanding which traits help animals cope could guide management.

The Channel Islands fox suggests that cognitive flexibility and problem-solving can be part of the survival toolkit – when ecosystems still offer enough resources to support those costly traits.

Yet the foxes’ low genetic diversity makes them vulnerable to disease and rapid environmental shifts, underscoring the need to protect habitats that sustain both bodies and brains.

Island syndrome, it turns out, is not a single script but a library of local stories. On five Channel Islands, small bodies came paired with relatively big brains. On the sixth, the opposite prevailed.

Together, those outcomes point to a simple conclusion with big implications: the brain follows the work that needs doing, and on islands, that work depends on the world outside the skull as much as on the DNA within it.

The study is published in the journal PLOS One.

—–

Like what you read? Subscribe to our newsletter for engaging articles, exclusive content, and the latest updates. 

Check us out on EarthSnap, a free app brought to you by Eric Ralls and Earth.com.

—–

News coming your way
The biggest news about our planet delivered to you each day
Subscribe